Watch CBS News

It's All About The Sample

By Kathy Frankovic, CBS News director of surveys


Why do pollsters make such a big deal about probability sampling?

You'd think that choosing people to interview should be easy. Take three of these, four of those, and two of the other. But it's not. Polls work because they rely on probability sampling, which ensures that all members of the sampling frame (the people being represented by the poll) have a measurable chance of being selected. (For instance, random digit dialing procedures select numbers from all existing numbers, and exit pollsters can talk with "every fourth voter" who leaves the polling place.) As far as possible, this takes the decision of whom to interview out of the hands of humans — no self-selection ("Please interview me!"), and no choosing people who are easiest to reach. Some famous polling mistakes bring that point home.

Early U.S. polling met with significant success until the 1948 election, when all of the pre-election polls went horribly wrong. They predicted a victory by New York State Republican Governor Thomas Dewey over incumbent Democratic President Harry Truman. In 1948, polling methodology routinely used "quota" samples, which gave interviewers the ability to choose respondents more or less on their own, with "quotas" for the number of men, women, young people, old people, "high" status and "low" status respondents.

The accuracy of polls conducted this way had been essentially unchallenged by the media and the public, although some government statisticians and some academics noted the possibility for error. So, after the mis-predicted 1948 election, one academic entity, the Social Science Research Council, created a blue-ribbon panel to review the polling process. It recommended the use of probability sampling at all levels of respondent selection, and the elimination of quotas.

There was, of course, another reason for this most famous of polling mistakes: simple overconfidence. From their work in the 1936, 1940 and 1944 presidential elections, pollsters believed there were few — if any — changes in public opinion that could be attributed to the fall campaign. Consequently, they assumed that Dewey's lead in the early fall could not be affected by anything that either he or Truman did. So they stopped polling two weeks before the election.

After the horrors of their 1948 error, pollsters switched to a probability-based process, and no longer left the choice of respondents to their interviewers. In addition, they decided to poll much closer to Election Day. Their poll predictions and reports for the 1952 election were both more accurate and more humble!

Yet the quota habit persisted elsewhere. In the 1992 British parliamentary election, polls erroneously predicted a Labour Party victory. In fact, the Conservative Party won by eight percentage points. As the U.S. Social Science Research Council did in 1948, the British Market Research Association conducted an investigation — and found similar problems with the sampling methods. Many British pollsters had relied on in-person interviews and used quota (not probability) sampling. Once again, interviewers chose whom to interview. After 1992, some British polling organizations adopted telephone polling, which made probability sampling of households and individuals much easier.

Exit polls are probably the most famous recent polling innovation. The first was conducted inadvertently in the United States by Ruth Clark, a well-known newspaper researcher who began her research career as an interviewer. In 1964, she was going door-to-door in Maryland, looking for voters to interview on the state's primary election day. Tired of walking, she decided to go to a polling place and talk with voters as they were leaving. As she later put it, "I told Lou [pollster Lou Harris] what I had done, and by the [Republican] California primary in June, the exit poll was put to full use." That day, Barry Goldwater voters dropped blue beans into a jar, while Nelson Rockefeller voters dropped red beans.

But exit polls also can fall victim to selection problems. The largest error in presidential election exit poll history occurred in 2004, when the disparity between the vote count and the exit poll exceeded six percentage points. And the source of the problem — as in 1948 and 1992 — came back to the issue of who was interviewed. Interviewers weren't supposed to use quotas, but many (especially younger) interviewers confronted difficulties at the polling place. Sometimes they did not interview the sample of voters the way they were supposed to, using probability selection (e.g., every second voter, or third voter, or 10th voter, depending on the total numbers of voters at the precinct). Instead, they interviewed whomever it was most convenient for them to interview (often people most like themselves). The result was very much the result that plagued the 1948 and 1992 quota-based systems.

Fixing this problem, once again, meant going back to the basics — especially meeting the rules of probability.

By Kathy Frankovic

View CBS News In
CBS News App Open
Chrome Safari Continue
Be the first to know
Get browser notifications for breaking news, live events, and exclusive reporting.